Experimental Psychology – PSY 270 010 20046

 

Review for Test on Chapter 11 and 12

 

 

Suppose you conduct an factorial experiment with a 3 by 4 design and both factors are between subjects. Assume you need 10 subjects per condition. How many subjects do you need?

 

3 by 4 means 12 conditions, so you you need 120 subjects.

 

Suppose, in a 3 by 4 factorial design, the first factor is within subjects and the second is between. If you need 10 subjects per condition, how many subjects do you need?

 

Because the second factor is between subjects and it has 4 levels, you need 40 subjects. Each one of the 40 subjects gives you an observation in each of the 3 levels of the within subjects factor, so you have 120 observations.

 

Suppose in a 3 by 4 factorial design, the first factor is between subjects and the second is within. If you need 10 subjects per condition, how many subject do you need?

 

Because the first factor is between subjects and it has 3 levels, you need 30 subjects. Each one of the 30 subjects gives you an observation in each of the 4 levels of the within subjects factor, so you have 120 observations.

 

Suppose in a 3 by 4 factorial design, both factors are within subjects. If you need 10 subjects per condition, how many subjects do you need?

 

You only need 10 subjects because each one participates in every condition. There are 12 condition, so you have a total of 120 observation.


 

Suppose some researchers in San Francisco are investigating the effects of reading different kinds of promotional material on people’s attitudes towards the value of education. They hypothesize that reading promotional material for Mensa (the organization for people with a genius IQ) might cause people to consider education more valuable than reading such material for joining a trade union. 

 

After they have randomly assigned each of a group of subjects to their two conditions, they notice that one group is entirely white subjects, whereas the other is all Asians. They believe that the effects they are trying to support could be different depending upon the race of the subject. Although such differences might be interesting in themselves, the researchers’ main concern is that they don’t want such differences to interfere with their ability to support their hypothesis.

 

Discuss what the researchers could do in this situation, and the relative advantages and disadvantages of the different approaches.

 

Re-randomize?

 

Should they repeat the randomization until the groups “look random”? No, there is no such thing as a “random group”. It is the process that needs to be random. But neither should the experiment be conducted without getting rid of the obvious confound that has inadvertently occured.

 

 

Use all the same race?

 

They could run the experiment with only whites or only Asians. That would remove any effect of race, but it would lower the generalizability of the results.

 

 

Block on race?

 

The best thing they could do is to take all of the whites and randomly assign each one to one of the two conditions, and then do the same thing for the Asians. This is called “blocking on race”. In each condition, there is an equal sized block of each race.

 

 

Crossing Factors:

 

Notice that this is the same thing as considering race to be a second factor in the study and “crossing” race with the promotional materials factor. Crossing two factors means forming conditions that consist of every possible combination of the levels of the two factors.

 

If there are two racial groups, and two levels of promotional materials, then you have a two by two design, and there are four conditions.

 

Whenever you cross factors, the number of conditions is the product of the number of levels of factors. Thus, if there are either a lot of factors, or a lot of levels for any of the factors, you can end up with a lot of conditions. If this happens, you can consider using “nesting” instead of “crossing”.

 

 

Nesting Factors:

 

Suppose your first factor (e.g., textbooks) has 9 levels and your second factor (ways of using the textbooks) has 3. Crossing the factors requires 27 conditions. If your design is between subjects, and you need at least 20 subjects in a condition, then you need 540 subjects.

 

Nesting means that you combine some levels of the first factor (the one with a lot of levels) with one of the levels of the other factor, and then combine some other levels of the first factor with the other levels of the second. But you don’t combine all with all.

 

Let’s call the nine levels of the first level, A1 through A9, and the levels of the second factor B1 through B3. Rather than crossing, and having all 27 conditions:

 

A1B1 A2B1     A3B1     A4B1     A5B1     A6B1     A7B1     A8B1     A9B1

A1B2 A2B2     A3B2     A4B2     A5B2     A6B2     A7B2     A8B2     A9B2

A1B3 A2B3     A3B3     A4B3     A5B3     A6B3     A7B3     A8B3     A9B3

 

you could nest like this, and have only 9 conditions:

 

A1B1   A2B1     A3B1

A4B2   A5B2     A6B2

A7B3   A8B3     A9B3

 

This would enable you to test the effects of the textbooks, and the effects of the methods to some extent. But there are a large number of ways that the textbooks could interact with the methods, and this nested design might show only some of them. And it might not be possible to tell whether an effect is an interaction or really a main effect.

 

For example, suppose that, in general, the first method is the worst, the second medium, and the third the best. Suppose that, for the most part, the text seems to make no difference. But suppose you also notice that when the first text is used, then you get just as good performance in B1 as in B3. Does text 1 improve learning only when combined with method 1? If so, then that is an interaction. Or would text 1 improve learning no matter what method it was combined with. In that case it is a main effect.